Philosophy Placement Data and Analysis: An Update (guest post by Carolyn Dicey Jennings)

Philosophy Placement Data and Analysis: An Update (guest post by Carolyn Dicey Jennings)


The following is a guest post* by Carolyn Dicey Jennings (UC Merced), who has led a team of academics in producing and organizing a trove of data related to the graduation and placement records of English-language philosophy Ph.D. programs (previously).  The team just published an update to its 2015 report, “Academic Placement Data and Analysis” (APDA). Among other things, the update contains the graduation and placement data for specific programs, some of which I’ve included below the post. Thanks to Professor Jennings and the rest of the team—Patrice Cobb, Chelsea Gordon, Bryan Kerster, Angelo Kyrilov, Evette Montes, Sam Spevack, David W. Vinson, and Justin Vlasits—for their work on this project.


An Update to Academic Placement Data and Analysis
by Carolyn Dicey Jennings

I am happy to report that the Academic Placement Data and Analysis (APDA) project has been able to release an update of its 2015 report, which includes program-specific placement rates. We decided not to include those in the original 2015 report because of errors we found just before its release. Those errors were in fact for records entered by placement officers and department chairs over the summer (e.g. missed placements), so we decided to spend the next six months or so checking the placement records in our database against other available sources, such as the PhilJobs Appointments page and program-specific placement pages. Having integrated that data, while also checking for duplicates and other errors, we feel confident enough in the data we have to release program-specific placements. This is a first for our discipline, and I am very grateful to all who have helped make it happen, including the American Philosophical Association (APA), all the APDA project personnel, and numerous people in philosophy who have helped the project in different ways (and me, when I needed it).

A first note: this does not mean that our database is error free. Only a week ago, long after removing duplicates in the database, Angelo Kyrilov noted that I was in the database twice—once for Boston College and once for Boston University. This was a coding error that was not caught in our system of checking for duplicates because one entry had my last name as “Jennings” and the other had my last name as “Dicey Jennings.” While we did check for misspellings of names, these were far enough apart in the alphabet to miss the duplication. I think it is a sign of the scientific nature of our project that the project head (me!) could have an incorrect entry. Yet, it is also a reminder that errors are inevitable in a project of this nature. We have made significant efforts to reduce and remove them, but when we add the option of individual editing in May we hope that still more will be noticed and removed.

So what did we find? One notable difference between this report and the one released in August is that we did not find a significant effect of gender on placement this time. There were some differences in how we made our comparisons—in the last report we grouped all but permanent in one category, comparing this to permanent placements. In this report we separated out temporary academic positions, nonacademic positions, and no reported positions (we have a small number of the latter two categories—251 total nonacademic positions and 204 graduates with no reported positions). We did this because we hope to expand our project to take better account of nonacademic positions. If we had used the previous model we would likely have found significance. In our database, 54% of the women have permanent academic positions, whereas only 49% of the men have permanent academic positions, and this difference is significant using a chi-squared test (p<.01). Yet, I was surprised that a number of programs (7, or around 7%) had no reported women graduates in the time period we looked at: 2012-2015. It is important to remember that disparity in philosophy is still high. (See this recent draft by Eric Schwitzgebel and myself for an in-depth accounting of that disparity.)

When reading our tables and graphs, keep in mind that 2015 is less-well represented in terms of number of graduates in our database: the 2015 graduates make up only 15% of the total graduates between 2012 and 2015 (the other three years each make up around the same proportion—28%). Since we did not add a time dimension to our analyses this year (but plan to do this in the future, as with the 2015 report), and since graduates of 2012 have had more time to secure permanent academic positions, those programs with fewer graduates from 2012 and more graduates from 2015 will likely appear to be worse off on placement relative to their peers than they actually are. (For this reason, I ordered the graduation year graphs by proportion of graduates from 2012.)

Also notable is that Value Theory remains the most popular of our first-listed area of specialization categories for this time period (2012-2015), with Language, Epistemology, Metaphysics, and Mind in second place, History and Traditions in third, and Science, Logic, and Math as the least populated of the categories. (This is excluding the “Unknown” category, which in fact beats Value Theory.) Nearly every program had some graduates with first-listed AOS in this category, with only 12 with no such graduates (excluding two programs for which we have only unknown AOS). Yet, Science, Logic, and Math was the category closest to significance, with more likelihood of those in our database with this AOS being reported as having a placement than not having a placement (in comparison with LEMM). (Value Theory and History and Traditions each had lower likelihoods for the academic positions and higher likelihoods for the nonacademic positions, but these values were not significant.)

Finally, we are still looking for the best way of capturing graduation data. Our current method is to take the mean for three external sources and then compare that to the numbers of graduates in our own database, taking the higher of these numbers. All three sources are good ones: the APA Guide to Graduate Programs, PhilJobs Appointments, and the Survey of Earned Doctorates (SED). Yet, there were lots of inconsistencies in the data. For example, in the same year a single program might have reported twice or three times the graduates in one source as in another (comparing PhilJobs and APA, since the data for both of these sources come from the departments themselves). This fact makes the search for reliable information about graduation numbers particularly tricky. If we were to use only SED data we might avoid some problems, but potentially fall prey to others, such as the fact that SED groups graduates by “field of doctorate” rather than department (and so might have higher or lower numbers for philosophy departments than they should, depending on how graduates see themselves). I am open to suggestions on this front. Needless to say, placement rates are sensitive to graduation numbers, so this is an important part of the project.

So what is the overall placement rate? Including graduates from all four years for 108 doctoral programs, and using external graduation data, 32% of graduates from 2012-2015 have by now found permanent academic positions and 68% have found academic positions of some kind. My supposition is that a large chunk of the remaining 32% are now in nonacademic positions. Yet our database only has record of a small proportion of these. For this reason, I am starting to think of the project somewhat differently, and will aim to do a better job collecting non-academic placements in 2016 by enabling individual reporting and editing of placement records. (I am supposing that placement officers and department chairs know less about these graduates.) We will begin thinking of how to categorize nonacademic positions so that we can, in future, have fewer reported unknowns and a better understanding of this large proportion of graduates.

Exciting developments are to come for APDA: we are going to send out a qualitative survey to all graduates in our database in May, reporting results in June. We are also going to add applications to the website that will allow users to interact more directly with the data, along with other website upgrades. If you have any feedback for the project, please let me know! We have turned off editing for now as we prepare for individual editing, but if you find any errors feel free to email me directly, preferably at the project email account.
APDA data cube


 

APDA 2015 update AOS

APDA 2015 update placement job typeAPDA 2015 update gender

See the rest of the data in the official APDA update.

Hedgehog Review
Subscribe
Notify of
guest

106 Comments
Oldest
Newest Most Voted
Inline Feedbacks
View all comments
gopher
gopher
8 years ago

Thanks so much for doing this (and thanks to your collaborators).

You say, “In our database, 54% of the women have permanent academic positions, whereas only 49% of the men have permanent academic positions.”
The data you give in the paper has permanent jobs for 457 out of 1168 men, which is 39%, and 228/466 women, which is 48%.

You don’t show the data broken down by gender *and* specialty together (so we could see how, e.g., LEMM men are doing compared to LEMM females). Will you be releasing that data?

Carolyn Dicey Jennings
Carolyn Dicey Jennings
Reply to  gopher
8 years ago

Hi gopher–the first statement covers the entire database of 4k plus people, whereas the latter statement is for 2012-2015 graduates only (under 2k). I should probably have looked at the latter when checking for significance in this post, since that is the better comparison with the analysis in the paper. My guess is that both would be significant if we grouped as we did for the 2015 report. On allowing for comparison between multiple data categories: yes, we will do that in the future. On AOS and gender: check out the paper that I wrote with Eric, which has this information (with data from 12/15): http://www.faculty.ucr.edu/~eschwitz/SchwitzPapers/WomenInPhil-160315b.pdf

gopher
gopher
Reply to  Carolyn Dicey Jennings
8 years ago

Right, I see the relevant part of your paper with Eric.
I was wondering, though, about how gender and AOS are related in the *placement* data. For example, women have a better placement rate than men — is this because they are more likely to be in value theory and value theory has a better placement rate? That sort of question.

Another Gopher,
You had to be another gopher? There are plenty of rodents. If you’re partial to Geomyoidea, couldn’t you be a kangaroo rat?

Carolyn Dicey Jennings
Carolyn Dicey Jennings
Reply to  gopher
8 years ago

Good questions–women are more likely to have a first-listed AOS in the Value Theory category, but that category actually had a slightly lower likelihood of having a recorded placement versus no recorded placement (not significant and not using external graduation numbers). We prioritized getting this report out before April 15th and, as last time, had the least time to work on the analysis side. Hopefully in the coming months as we think through these issues more we can come up with interesting analyses to run on the data.

Joel
Joel
8 years ago

April 15th. Gosh. Well, Hopefully this can help some folks to make better informed decisions.

Just casually, I’m noticing that there are major differences between schools that are grouped together in Leiter’s recent rankings. Group 4, for example. Grad applicants, I beg you, don’t make the mistake I did and prioritize prestige over actual results.

recent grad
recent grad
8 years ago

How were so-called ‘Continental” dissertations classified? Is “Foucault on Knowledge” classified as a LEMMing diss, tradition diss, or unknown diss?

Carolyn Dicey Jennings
Carolyn Dicey Jennings
Reply to  recent grad
8 years ago

Off hand, I would probably group this as having a focus on Foucault, and thus Continental/History and Traditions, rather than having a focus on Epistemology, and thus LEMM. (Similarly for “Aristotle on Ethics.”) All AOS entries and categorizations were checked by both myself and Justin Vlasits (a graduate student in philosophy at Berkeley), but graduate dissertations were assigned AOS terms by myself and Evette Montes (who was more likely to select “unknown” if she couldn’t find this information on a c.v.).

recent grad
recent grad
Reply to  Carolyn Dicey Jennings
8 years ago

Thanks. And thanks for putting this together.

APDA fan
APDA fan
8 years ago

Carolyn,

This is an absolutely fantastic service, and I am really grateful to you and your team for doing all this.

Anyway, I just wanted to briefly follow up on the comment from gopher. Judging from the numbers in the report, 39% of men got permanent positions, while 48% of women got permanent positions. This difference is highly statistically significant, chi-squared (1, N= 1634)= 11.485, p= 0.0007.

The actual report uses a more complex analysis that breaks down the people who did not get permanent positions into a number of different categories (none, non-academic, etc.). I don’t mean to criticize that analysis, but it would be a shame to lose sight of the straightforward fact that these data show that men are significantly less likely to get permanent positions than women are.

Carolyn Dicey Jennings
Carolyn Dicey Jennings
Reply to  APDA fan
8 years ago

Thanks for this–just a follow up: these data do not include external graduation numbers, and the external graduation numbers I have do not include gender, and this information may well have an impact on the analysis. In other words, it may not be that men are less likely to get permanent positions but that men are more likely to get temporary academic positions and women are more likely to be in the unknown placement category. This conversation reminds me that SED has gender data, so we might be able to look into this after all. (But not today!)

APDA fan
APDA fan
Reply to  Carolyn Dicey Jennings
8 years ago

Carolyn,

Thanks so much for this response — and thanks once again for everything you’ve done to create this fantastic report!

Another gopher
Another gopher
8 years ago

Carolyn,
Thanks to you and your collaborators for your hard work and the update.

Your initial 2015 report states, “In terms of differences, the odds of having a permanent (versus temporary) academic placement are 85% greater
for females as compared to males.” (p. 11)

However your update does not compare the odds of permanent (versus temporary) placement by gender. It only reports “Permanent academic placement relative to no placement” (pp. 8-9), “Temporary academic placement relative to no placement” (p. 9), “nonacademic placement relative to no placement” (p. 9), and “Both temporary academic placement and nonacademic placement relative to no placement” (p. 9)

Can you analyze and report the statistics/odds for the permanent (versus temporary) placement intersect by gender, as in the first report? This is a highly relevant intercept that came out highly significant in the original report.

p. 7 of the the update reports 225/446 women placed in permanent academic positions (48%) compared to 457/1168 (39%) for men. On p. 12 of the original 2015 report 138/257 (53%) of women placed compared to 269/681 (39%) of men. These figures are similar. It is hard to believe that an 85% advantage would retreat to a statistical null result after a one year update. The statistical probability of a retreat to null result is vanishingly small given the p-value (p<.0001) in the original report (a 1 in 10,000 chance).

Stacey Goguen
8 years ago

Just want to say thanks to you and everyone who’s worked on the project. And I am looking forward to future data on non-academic job placement!

Philippe Lemoine
8 years ago

Thanks for the update, it’s very interesting, as per usual.

I’m pretty sure the only reason you didn’t find a significant effect of gender this year is that, as another gopher already noted, you didn’t perform the same analysis as in your last report, i. e. you didn’t compare the odds of reporting a permanent academic position vs a temporary academic placement. I have little doubt that, if you run that regression instead, you will find that gender has a significant effect. I also suspect that the effects of AOS will become significant and that some of them will be reversed.

Also, I seem to remember that you were using a multilevel regression model before, which doesn’t seem to be the case this year. I’m inclined to think a multilevel regression model is more appropriate here, but I don’t think that is what explain the non-significance of gender. You also used to control for the year of graduation, which strikes me as a rather important factor to control for.

I hope you will release the data at some point so that everyone can play with them and run their own analyses.

Carolyn Dicey Jennings
Carolyn Dicey Jennings
Reply to  Philippe Lemoine
8 years ago

Hi Philippe–You and anyone else can use the data that has been made public here: http://faculty.ucr.edu/~eschwitz/SchwitzAbs/WomenInPhil.htm . This is from a pull I did in December 2015, not using a SQL Query but an Excel match function on the tables, which is more likely to lead to errors of various kinds. We will have to rewrite our SQL Query to better represent the different types of placements. (It currently implicitly prioritizes permanent over temporary and academic over nonacademic, such that if someone first got a permanent academic job but now has a nonacademic job we listed them as permanent. We will change that.) We will put up a public csv file with the website upgrades (as well as the reports), but that is not our top priority right now. I suspect we will complete those website upgrades in May or June of this year.

Carolyn Dicey Jennings
Carolyn Dicey Jennings
Reply to  Carolyn Dicey Jennings
8 years ago

It is Appendix 2. Coding: 1=men, 2-women; 1=LEMM, 2=Value, 3=Hist, 4=Sci, 5=Unknown; >0=Placement (for both Perm and Temp).

Another gopher
Another gopher
Reply to  Carolyn Dicey Jennings
8 years ago

It should not be your audience’s responsibility to carry out relevant analyses. Research ethics require researchers to present all relevant statistical analyses, not present data analysis selectively. This is to ensure that data are not misleadingly represented to the public. Your post states, “One notable difference between this report and the one released in August is that we did not find a significant effect of gender on placement this time.” Several commenters have noted this does not appear to be true, and it should not be asserted given that some relevant analyses from the 2015 report were not performed in the update. In order to justifiably assert whether a significant effect of gender was or was not found, one must perform and present statistical analyses of all relevant intercepts.

babygirl
babygirl
Reply to  Another gopher
8 years ago

“Research ethics require researchers to present all relevant statistical analyses, not present data analysis selectively.”

I can’t think of a way this statement could be interpreted such that it is even remotely plausible.

Another gopher
Another gopher
Reply to  babygirl
8 years ago

Non-selectivity/completeness in statistical reporting is a widely recognized part of research ethics.

“Like any other field, there are ethics in statistics that need to be followed by a researcher so that only the truth is reported and there is no misrepresentation of the data…It is relatively simple to manipulate and hide data, projecting only what one desires and not what the numbers actually speak, thus giving birth to the famous phrase “Lies, damned lies and statistics”…Ethics in statistics are very important during data representation as well. Numbers don’t lie but their interpretation and representation can be misleading. For example, after a broad survey of many customers, a company might decide to publish and make available only the numbers and figures that reflect well on the company and either totally neglect or not give due importance to other figures.”
https://explorable.com/ethics-in-statistics

babygirl
babygirl
Reply to  Another gopher
8 years ago

That’s not what you were talking about.

This quote is about completeness in *reporting results*, not about carrying out “all relevant statistical analyses” … in what sense “relevant”? And why would that be her responsibility? Of course researchers are selective about what analyses to perform, according to what they judge is best, what they think is interesting or exciting, etc.

And I still find what you quote there problematic —in some sense, “manipulating data” is just what researchers do.

another gopher
another gopher
Reply to  Another gopher
8 years ago

It is relevant in the sense that the study found an 85% gender advantage last time on the intercept, but Carolyn did not test it this time before publicly claiming a null result for gender advantage.

Carolyn Dicey Jennings
Carolyn Dicey Jennings
Reply to  Another gopher
8 years ago

“Your post states, `One notable difference between this report and the one released in August is that we did not find a significant effect of gender on placement this time.’ Several commenters have noted this does not appear to be true”

I missed the commenters who noted that we did in fact find a significant effect of gender on placement this time. I did notice that some commenters agree with what I say in my post–that we would have likely found significance if we had run different analyses, collapsing across distinctions. I, like babygirl, disagree with your phrasing of what is required of researchers, but perhaps you could rephrase it? In any case, we presented all of the analyses that we ran.

Eric
Eric
Reply to  Carolyn Dicey Jennings
8 years ago

Hi Carolyn,

Here’s on thing that I would think is not permissible for statistical researches to do.

1. A hypothesis H0 has been found to have statistically significant support for rejection.
2. The researchers in question run a study, and claim that they failed to replicate the finding in favor of rejectding H0. Some would claim this statement “`One notable difference between this report and the one released in August is that we did not find a significant effect of gender on placement this time.’ ” comes awfully close to that.
3. What the researchers actually did was to, for not particularly good reason, restrict the power of their study by breaking the hypothesis in subcategories in such that a way that it is a priori predictable that the evidence for rejecting H0 would less likely to be found.

Carolyn Dicey Jennings
Carolyn Dicey Jennings
Reply to  Eric
8 years ago

This is better, Eric. Yet I disagree that 2 and 3 are an accurate portrayal of the above.

2. We did not aim to replicate the earlier finding and I don’t think my claim makes it sound like we did. I purposefully avoided saying what the gender effect consisted of in the two cases because the cases were different–our question was different. Note that the claim you quote is immediately followed by the explanation that I suspect the superficially apparent difference is because of a change in analyses and that gender would have had a significant impact if we had performed the other analysis. (This is something I realized while writing this post, late in the evening of April 15th. If I had realized it earlier, we might have run a different analysis to check. But we did not know the results of the analysis until the afternoon of the 14th, at which point I completed the report. We had run the analyses earlier but we found an issue with the query, which needed to be updated along with a host of other tasks.)

3. I provided a reason–our dataset has more categories than permanent and temporary. If anything, we probably should have divided these out in August, but in that case too we had very little time for analysis. By happy accident we found an effect of gender. It was like discovering penicillin. (Well, in fact, I had found a difference between permanent and temporary placements in past analyses, before APDA: http://www.newappsblog.com/2014/06/job-placements-2011-2014-first-report.html . Interestingly, those who may have been disappointed that we found an effect of gender last August, when I had found no effect of gender in 2014 and earlier, did not use the occasion of their disappointment to publicly question my intentions and values. I appreciate that restraint.)

Over the next month or so we will think more about the data and what we should do next and hopefully release a more reflective set of analyses then. If you have feedback on how we might improve the project, please let me know.

Eric
Eric
Reply to  Eric
8 years ago

Well, I would say the quoted sentence sounds an awful lot like “we tried but failed to replicate an earlier finding” in which case 2 and 3 would definitely be accurate. I think that’s probably what’s got a few people talking about research standards. Your explanation makes sense though. I should have looked for myself at how the sentence occurred in context.

In response to your request for feedback, an obvious suggesting would be to check and see if the previous finding can in fact be replicated. Can you reject the null hypothesis that

M/F is uncorrelated with (permanent hire)/~(permanent hire)

prima facie, that would seem to be both the question most people would be interested in, and the one that your study has the greatest power to answer.

Carolyn Dicey Jennings
Carolyn Dicey Jennings
Reply to  Eric
8 years ago

The graduate student who runs the analyses, Patrice Cobb, has been working on this analysis since yesterday, around the time the post went up. We will post her results here or at the APA Blog when she is ready.

APDA fan
APDA fan
Reply to  Carolyn Dicey Jennings
8 years ago

I am one of the commenters above who said that these data provide strong evidence that men are less likely than women to receive permanent positions. I am disappointed that the interesting discussion we began there has degenerated into this weird debate about whether Carolyn did something wrong. In my view, Carolyn did not do anything wrong, but either way, this whole debate seems like a pointless distraction. Surely, the important question is not about whether Carolyn did something wrong but rather about what these data actually show.

gopher
gopher
Reply to  APDA fan
8 years ago

I’m with APDA fan.
Carolyn is obviously doing a huge service. It is very odd to suspect she is doing it out of bad will. And she is obviously interested in doing more, so someone who wants to see other analysis only has to make a good case for it. Come on.

Philippe Lemoine
Reply to  Carolyn Dicey Jennings
8 years ago

Thanks! I think I’ll wait for the cleaned-up csv file. I was kind of hoping of using that opportunity to learn R, which I have been meaning to do for a while now. I also really should read that paper you wrote with Eric Schwitzgebel.

Philippe Lemoine
Reply to  Philippe Lemoine
8 years ago

Just a quick note to make clear that, if I have no doubt that gender would still have a significant effect if we performed the same analysis as you did last year, it isn’t because I have some magical insight into that issue. It’s because except for the people that you have recently added in the database, which only represent a small proportion of the total, plus a few corrections here and there, the data are essentially the same as before. Hence, given how small the p-value was with the original data, for the effect no longer to be significant, it would have to be the case that, among the people you recently added to the database, men were *overwhelmingly* more likely to have found a permanent academic position rather than a temporary academic position, enough to compensate the pattern in the original data, which just doesn’t seem plausible at all. In fact, for all we know, if we performed the same analysis as last year on this new dataset, it’s entirely possible that the effect of gender is even stronger.

Alexander Guerrero
8 years ago

Thanks for all of your work on this. I have a question about the data. The placement rate info is striking. Just out of curiosity, I went to look at the University of Washington placement page, since that department came out with the highest proportion of its graduates from 2012-2015 in “permanent academic” positions. But their placement record seems very different than the data reported here. It seems maybe 4/9 of the graduates are in “permanent academic” positions. But perhaps there is some significant difference in how that term is being used by this report? Did you run checks with the publicly available data, or did you rely entirely on self-reported data? Just curious what explains this apparent discrepancy. (I find the apparent discrepancy a bit troubling given that it is the only page I checked…) Here is the UW placement page: https://phil.washington.edu/graduate-job-placements

Carolyn Dicey Jennings
Carolyn Dicey Jennings
Reply to  Alexander Guerrero
8 years ago

Thanks for this, Alex! I started looking into this for University of Washington. It looks like we match on the TTs and 1 temporary from 2013, but the other positions listed are as permanent in our database (e.g. permanent lectureship). I don’t know if their positions are permanent or not, but if a mistake was made it would either be by the placement officer/department chair or by project personnel assigned to check the data for this program. It may be that we need to provide more guidance on what can count as permanent or temporary here, but we had intended to capture different forms of permanent contracts that may not be TT contracts. Thank you for bringing this to my attention, I will think it through some more and discuss with the other APDA members.

Carolyn Dicey Jennings
Carolyn Dicey Jennings
Reply to  Carolyn Dicey Jennings
8 years ago

Also, I should say that I suspect that two of the positions in 2012 and two of the positions in 2014 are not permanent, and if I had been the one checking that program I would have changed/entered them as temporary academic positions. But I was not able to check all of the programs myself. I tried to have graduate students do this, but in the end our undergraduate RA checked these pages. In the future, I might need to work more closely with undergraduate RAs and/or assign people with more background knowledge to this type of task.

Alexander Guerrero
Reply to  Carolyn Dicey Jennings
8 years ago

Thanks for the response. I think the big worry is if some departments are reporting information one way, and others are reporting it another way. That seems likely to be happening here. I think there is also a worry if “permanent” positions are permanent only in that the person in the position can “permanently” reapply for the same short-term contract position, or if they are in a similar relatively insecure academic position. Given that there is likely to be significant variation here in the details, I’d think it might be better to go for a sharp distinction between TT/”Lecturer in the UK” kinds of protected employment positions, and other kinds of academic positions.

Carolyn Dicey Jennings
Carolyn Dicey Jennings
Reply to  Alexander Guerrero
8 years ago

This is a good suggestion–let me talk it over with the others. I do remember that some program representatives described cases like this one: a university technically only hires faculty on an adjunct basis but all faculty are treated as though permanent (I think the Naval Academy does this, for example). I thought it made sense to group those with permanent, but we might need to revisit this to help those entering data do so as accurately as possible.

Carolyn Dicey Jennings
Carolyn Dicey Jennings
Reply to  Alexander Guerrero
8 years ago

This information might be helpful: I looked at the proportion of those with permanent jobs listed as TT versus other (e.g. lectureships) and found that around 11% of those with permanent positions who graduated 2012-2015 have a non-TT permanent job (such as a lectureship). This number is around 8% for graduates of all years.

MG
MG
Reply to  Carolyn Dicey Jennings
7 years ago

I think it’s very important not to lump non-TT permanent positions together with simple temporary positions, merely on the basis of the fact that they aren’t TT. I’m not convinced that it’s necessary to treat TT as a special category of its own, but if it is, then at least there should be a division along these lines: TT, permanent non-TT, temporary.

new grad student
new grad student
8 years ago

Hi Carolyn,

Thanks so much for putting this data together. I really enjoyed looking through the charts, and found them very helpful.

I’m somewhat confused about how the job placements are being reported, and I’m wondering if you might be able to bring me clarity. It’s possible that the answers are around and I’ve overlooked them, and if so I’m sorry that I didn’t find them.

Two questions:

(1) Does the data on placements focus strictly on first-year placements (i.e. what jobs grad students got the fall after finishing their PhD’s)? Or is other placement data included?

(2) I noticed what appeared to be a couple of discrepancies and I wonder if it indicates that I’m misunderstanding the results or the way the data is reported. According to the analysis, Washington (I’m assuming this is St. Louis) placed 100% of their PhD students in academic jobs. But according to Washington’s website, in 2015 a graduate went and worked for the military in “an analytical position” (https://philosophy.artsci.wustl.edu/graduate/placement/academic-placement-record). Similarly, the analysis says that UT Austin placed 100% in academic jobs, but their website reports that in 2012 a student graduated and became an “artificial intelligence developer” (http://www.utexas.edu/cola/philosophy/graduate/placement2/byyear.php). Are these minor errors, or am I misunderstanding either a) the nature of these jobs, or b) the study results?

Thanks again, and I certainly understand if you aren’t able to answer my questions at this time.

Carolyn Dicey Jennings
Carolyn Dicey Jennings
Reply to  new grad student
8 years ago

1) In this case, it is any placement we have for the graduate, starting with first permanent placement, then first temporary placement, then nonacademic placement (except in the model, in which case it was first permanent placement, then either temporary or nonacademic or both).

2) Washington St Louis is actually shortened to Wash U. I looked back to our notes on UT Austin and it looks like the placement page was down when we tried to check it. This is the only such case in which a placement page was temporarily down (and if I have noticed this in the notes I would have looked to see if the page was up before releasing this report), but our records should have been checked against PhilJobs in this case. Here is what we have for UT Austin: 2 permanent and one temporary academic position for 2012 grads (SED reports 3 grads, PhilJobs reports 2, so we assumed 3 grads); 3 permanent positions for 2013 grads (APA reports 4 grads, SED reports 1 grad, PhilJobs reports 2 grads, so we assumed 3 grads); 2 permanent and 1 temporary positions for 2014 grads (APA reports 1 grad, SED reports 3 grads, PhilJobs reports 3 grads, we assumed 3 grads); 2 permanent positions for 2015 grads (PhilJobs reports 1 grad, we assumed 2 grads). I can see from the placement page that there are far more graduates than this and we will have to update our numbers. As you can see from Appendix B, this program did not update entries with us in summer 2015.

some person or other
some person or other
8 years ago

Thanks for all your hard work at this.
One thing that prospective grad students might want to keep in mind is that the data you used will probably be at least somewhat misleading (not saying you should have done anything differently, as I don’t know how this would be avoided!) depending on whether, in a given program, it is advantageous or not to stay enrolled as a grad student when one doesn’t secure tt employment. I’m not sure how this would affect things, but I know that at some places there is lots of teaching available for grad students and so it can often be in someone’s interests to refrain from graduating for as long as possible while trying to secure a job, whereas in other places there is very little opportunity for getting continuing funding as a grad student past the fifth or sixth year. (Another thing that might affect this, possibly in the other direction, is that it is easier for some universities to employ their recent phds as lecturers than others.) I have no idea how people should think about this when reading your data, but I suspect given the relatively short window of time you are focusing on that it probably makes a difference somehow!

Also, I just want to note that I had a similar worry to Alex’s, and I’m also just curious generally because (of course, this could just be an interesting discovery!) the overall permanent job placement rate seems very high to me given how crappy the job market seems to actual candidates. (Also, I know in some ways things were worse this year, but many of the programs that appear to normally place around 50% of candidates in tt jobs placed zero people this year…) I was expecting a far lower placement rate overall into permanent jobs and I wonder whether Alex’s observation about Washington might generalize to other places as well…

Carolyn Dicey Jennings
Carolyn Dicey Jennings
Reply to  some person or other
8 years ago

In the past, before the creation of APDA, I reported an overall placement rate based on those who were new to the job market or just graduating, but this placement rate covers graduates from 2012 to 2015 who have gotten a job by sometime between the summer of 2015 and now, depending on when that program was checked. It may nonetheless be overly high if it turns out that there are more errors of the type discovered by Alex. It does not tell us whether the placement rate has gotten worse over time, but that is something we should look into.

Carolyn Dicey Jennings
Carolyn Dicey Jennings
8 years ago

Here is a csv file with the program specific information in case that is helpful for checking data: https://www.dropbox.com/s/51r5kyv1h4ojdfk/APDACheck.csv?dl=0

Carolyn Dicey Jennings
Carolyn Dicey Jennings
8 years ago
postdoc
postdoc
8 years ago

So, looking at the table starting page 16, 33% of PhDs graduating between 2012 and 2015 have permanent jobs, but 11% of these are not TT jobs (if I understand the above post correctly). So, 22% have TT jobs. That number does look somewhat realistic, but I suspect there is still inflation going on due to various factors. I think the most obvious factor that could be inflating this percentage is a selection bias.

On page 6 it says,

‘Programs were included only if we could verify both placement records and graduation numbers. Placement records were verified through placement pages and the PhilJobs Appointments page, although in a few cases we relied on placement officers or department chairs who updated the data for their own programs (see above).’

However, there is an obvious desire for programs with bad placements to hide this fact or at least not advertise it. So, I am worried the data excludes a lot of programs that would drag the 22% down significantly.

I suspect the real percentage is in the teens.

What do people think? Is this a real concern?

Derek Bowman
Derek Bowman
Reply to  postdoc
8 years ago

I think it’s a real concern; in general I still think it would be a mistake to rely too heavily on these numbers in evaluating specific programs, just because most of the absolute numbers are so low. I’ll leave it to Prof. Jennings and others to attend to technical matters of statistical significance, but the fact is if many programs have a dozen or less graduates over the measured time period then the percentages are very susceptible to dramatic statistical swings, not only because of reporting/coding but because of idiosyncratic factors relating to particular candidates. As the numbers grow over time this problem can be lessened, but only at the cost of hiding relevant information about how a program and/or the market has changed over longer periods of time.

But I do think as it improves it will continue to be a more and more accurate picture of the health of the profession as a profession tied to university employment.

One other worry about the current numbers is the (unless I missed it) lack of information about full-time vs part-time vs multiple-part-time employment. Temporary full-time employment is a very different thing from temporary part-time employment. I haven’t delved into the current report carefully, so I may have missed it. But I suspect that full-timers are more likely to be reported under temporary employment, while part-timers are more likely to be in the unknown/no information category. If that’s true, it should cause us to be cautious about our assumptions about how many of those unknowns are in full-time non-academic employment.

GF-A
GF-A
Reply to  postdoc
8 years ago

You write: “but 11% of these are not TT jobs (if I understand the above post correctly). So, 22% have TT jobs.” I’m not sure that’s right. What Prof. Jennings’ above post says is “around 11% of those *with permanent positions* who graduated 2012-2015 have a non-TT permanent job” (my emphasis) So I think it’s 11% of those 33%, not 11% of the total population. So, if I’ve understood correctly, about 29.4% of all PhDs graduating between 2012–2015 has a TT job.

postdoc
postdoc
Reply to  GF-A
7 years ago

Yea, I think you’re correct. Thanks!

postdoc
postdoc
Reply to  GF-A
7 years ago

With the selection bias I am speaking of, where programs with bad placements don’t have data available, I am still thinking the real percentage is quite a bit lower than 29%.

Poke
Poke
8 years ago

So, I guess I’ll go ahead and poke at the elephant in the room. It’s already been mentioned how dramatically PGR rank and placement come apart. Let’s look at specifics. None of the PGR top 3 break into the top 20 (the top 20!) in placement. Princeton and NYU both narrowly miss the top 20. Rutgers just barely breaks the top 35. Further, it’s not just that ‘continental’ or historically-oriented programs are managing to tie down their niche and somehow get great placement that way. UNC, Harvard, Indiana, and Michigan are, like the PGR top-3, all largely analytic programs, but they all fare much better than the top-3 in terms of TT-placement.

I do appreciate that this placement ranking does not take into account the many distinguishing characteristics of jobs, e.g., teaching load, type of institution. And I appreciate that a 3-year time span offers a relatively small sample of grads. These are all very important caveats. But still, this report seems to make pretty clear that prospective grads should not make decisions just based on the PGR. Thanks to Carolyn and her amazing team, they now don’t have to.

humean skeptic
humean skeptic
Reply to  Poke
7 years ago

Right, but we have no idea whether these programs make people more place-able or just admit more place-able people. So we have no idea what role this information should play in people’s decisions about where to go to graduate school.

Poke
Poke
Reply to  humean skeptic
7 years ago

It’s true that initial place-ability of incoming students isn’t something that’s been accounted for here. But it’s not true that we have “no idea” whether there is any influence from the department. It would be quite shocking if the training, mentoring, professional guidance, and grad climate one is exposed to for 5-8 of one’s formative professional years had *no* impact on one’s ability to produce good work and one’s ability to then parlay that work into a sell-able job dossier. It would be really strange to be skeptical on that point.

Also, I’m assuming that students at (say) UCLA, Berkeley, NYU, Rutgers, Harvard, Princeton, UNC, and Michigan all start grad school as roughly equally “place-able.” If that’s true, and if department training has no impact, they should all end up as equally *placing,* but they don’t. So it seems reasonable to think that department climate, mentoring, financial support, etc. help explain some of the difference.

humean skeptic
humean skeptic
Reply to  Poke
7 years ago

It would be quite shocking if the training, mentoring, professional guidance, and grad climate one is exposed to for 5-8 of one’s formative professional years had *no* impact on one’s ability to produce good work and one’s ability to then parlay that work into a sell-able job dossier. It would be really strange to be skeptical on that point.

Oh, of course. But unless we know how place-able they start out, we have no idea how much impact the various different programs had.

I’m assuming that students at (say) UCLA, Berkeley, NYU, Rutgers, Harvard, Princeton, UNC, and Michigan all start grad school as roughly equally “place-able.”

Well, you can assume that; I’m just noting that there’s no evidence for it. I mean, I could assume that each of those programs adds roughly equally to students’ ability to produce good work and ability to parlay that work into a sell-able job dossier, and then we’d have to conclude that some of the programs start off with much more place-able students than others. But neither assumption is warranted, really. We just have no idea how much each factor contributes.

postdoc
postdoc
Reply to  Poke
7 years ago

There is definitely prestige bias of some degree.

I mean it doesn’t take much investigation to see quite a few people getting great jobs from top top programs who do not have otherwise impressive CVs.

There are also other studies on prestige bias in academia. See for example,

http://philosopherscocoon.typepad.com/blog/2016/03/a-depressing-article-on-prestige-bias-along-with-some-possibly-helpful-advice.html

Jared Riggs
Jared Riggs
Reply to  Poke
7 years ago

Unless I’m missing something, the way programs are ranked by placement takes into account only the proportion of graduates who have attained permanent academic positions. I think this gives a somewhat distorted picture of things, for a couple of reasons.

First, compare the temporary positions of graduates from NYU and Minnesota, which are closely ranked on this data. NYU has 38% of graduates in temporary positions, and Minnesota has 46%. But NYU’s temporary positions seem to me a little better, intrinsically and instrumentally for the purposes of securing future employment, than Minnesota’s. The first three temporary positions listed on the placement page for 2015 NYU graduates are a UNC posdoc, an Antwerp postdoc, and the Harvard Society of Fellows. The first three temporary positions listed on the placement page for 2015 Minnesota graduates are Anoka Ramsey Community College, Hennepin Community College, and a postdoc at Kansas City University of Medicine and Biosciences. I’m not sure about other potential grad students, but I would prefer a UNC postdoc to a temporary position at Anoka Ramsey Community College.

Second, as you mention, the data doesn’t distinguish between the quality of jobs. Minnesota’s two 2015 placements into permanent positions at Minneapolis Community and Technical College count for as much as NYU’s TT placements into Indiana and USC. Again, prospective grad students might prefer a TT job at Indiana to a permanent position at Minneapolis Community and Technical College.

Third, since the data only goes back a few years, we might suspect that the programs that have placed people into relatively prestigious postdocs will move up the rankings as those people move from their postdocs into permanent positions. Programs who have placed people into relatively unprestigious postdocs and into temporary community college positions will probably not see as many students move from those positions into permanent positions.

So, although this data is indeed really helpful, and provides some information that’s just absent from the PGR, I don’t think we should be too impressed by the apparent disparity between PGR rank and ADPA rank, in large part because the APDA “ranking” ignores important qualitative factors in placement that would be highly relevant to one’s decision about where to pursue a PhD (but I don’t mean to say that it’s blameworthy for doing so, or anything). So I’m not really convinced, based only on this data, that prospective grad students who looked only at the PGR would be basing their decision on information that doesn’t correlate well with quality of placement.

Poke
Poke
Reply to  Jared Riggs
7 years ago

Hi Jared,

Yes, of course, I agree that it very much matters what kinds of jobs we’re talking about and what one’s long-term prospects of employment are. But, I still think PGR can lead you astray here (though probably not re: Minnesota vs. NYU, as you point out). Several of the UCs, Michigan, and Harvard all do better than NYU, Princeton, and Rutgers on tenure-track employment, and I’m assuming their temporary placements of the former are roughly similar to the latter’s in terms of quality (though I have not checked that). So, it still seems to me that if a prospective were to naively think, “NYU has better placement than Princeton, Princeton has better placement than MIT, MIT has better placement than Berkeley,” etc., it looks like they could go astray, at least if TT placement in 3 years is what they’re interested in and *even* if decent non-TT short-term placement is also what they’re interested in. Of course, the fullest picture of employment prospects would look at both quality of jobs and long-term prospects, and no one currently has that data collected, but given the data we have so far, it does not seem to me that differences between PGR rankings should be taken very seriously.

I guess here’s where you probably won’t go too far wrong in using PGR as a guide to placement — if you ignore fine distinctions between rankings but treat (say) the top 15 as one band of prestige, the next 15 as the next band of prestige, etc. But if you think the PGR difference between (say) Rutgers and UNC is indicative of placement, you could be in for a nasty surprise when you’re one of the 1/3 of Rutgers grads who in a 3-year span don’t get any kind of academic placement, even of a temporary kind.

Carolyn Dicey Jennings
Carolyn Dicey Jennings
Reply to  Poke
7 years ago

Two things:

1) the sorting of programs into proportion of permanent placements is not intended to be a ranking. It is not obvious to me that we would want to sort of that criteria alone for a ranking, but sorting by multiple criteria is visually confusing, which is why I chose to sort by a single variable. My hope is that sorting the programs helps you to look for trends and to explore the programs in more depth, relative to other programs.

2) “Minnesota’s two 2015 placements into permanent positions at Minneapolis Community and Technical College count for as much as NYU’s TT placements into Indiana and USC. Again, prospective grad students might prefer a TT job at Indiana to a permanent position at Minneapolis Community and Technical College.” This may be true, but I think we should be careful not to import the PGR into all of our value judgements–the PGR says nothing about whether working at one program is better than working at another. (I grant that you may not be doing this, but others have.) Given this, how should we measure one job against another? Is a community college job worse than a research-focused job? For almost every measure I can think of (happiness, productivity, etc.) this would depend on the person. For some, the pressure of research-focused jobs brings misery, whereas others thrive in this environment. For some a high teaching load is exhausting, whereas other love teaching more than any other part of the job. Some enjoy teaching the best prepared students, whereas others find meaning in teaching those from underprivileged backgrounds with less polish. (I know many people who primarily see themselves as teachers, and who have thought this way since before entering graduate school.) My plan is to incorporate Carnegie Classifications as soon as we are able, which will help you to distinguish programs that place in R1s versus programs that place in community colleges, but I will resist treating one of these categories as inherently better than the other. Here is an anecdote: I have a fantastic job–a 2:1 teaching load in which I determine the courses I teach (a good fit for me), amazing and supportive colleagues, truly impressive graduate students (our placement record seems pretty good for a brand new program, although I do not know how cognitive science graduates do generally–of 12 grads so far there are 3 TTs, 3 postdocs, 2 other temp, 2 industry, 1 works in education, and 1 is unknown), an undergraduate population who are invested in their education and for whom an education here makes a real difference, and a location near Yosemite National Park and other beautiful natural areas. Yet, I work at an R2 (as determined by the most recent Carnegie Classification release) that most in philosophy wouldn’t recognize and I work in a cognitive science program, which wouldn’t be a good fit for just anyone. I would love to be able to say something about which programs send graduates to the best jobs, but I am not interested in importing what I think is an unhealthy obsession with prestige in determining what the best jobs might be. I might be able to get at this with the qualitative survey–I will discuss this with those working on the survey (Gordon, Spevack, and Vinson).

Poke
Poke
Reply to  Carolyn Dicey Jennings
7 years ago

Hi Carolyn,

I agree with all your points here. I realize you did not intend the ordering w.r.t. TT-placement to constitute a ranking (but I was considering what might happen if one were to treat it that way). Of course non-TT placement is also very important.

I also agree that it is silly to assume that everyone wants a research-intensive job. I was admittedly thinking just of the person who wants such a job and was ignoring the many, many people who don’t, and you’re very right to call me out on that!

Thanks again for your marvelous work on this. So invaluable!

pachydermist
pachydermist
7 years ago

Another elephant in the room to poke at: there are several programs that graduated zero women during the 3-year span. Some of those programs had low numbers of graduates, so the sample is too small to be meaningful. But check out Miami. 15 grads and no women!

the Onion Man
the Onion Man
7 years ago

I love this. Quantitative evidence indicates that recent female graduates outperform recent male graduates in terms of permanent full-time academic jobs (you know, the thing that the overwhelming majority of people start Ph.D. programs in order to obtain). And yet the nitpicking starts instantly, because it contravenes the expected narrative.

slow clap

APDA fan
APDA fan
Reply to  the Onion Man
7 years ago

I would not describe this discussion in quite that way. You are right to say that ‘Quantitative evidence indicates that recent female graduates outperform recent male graduates in terms of permanent full-time academic jobs,’ but it does not seem that there is anyone in this thread who is specifically arguing for the opposite view. On the contrary, the original post explicitly says:

In our database, 54% of the women have permanent academic positions, whereas only 49% of the men have permanent academic positions, and this difference is significant using a chi-squared test (p<.01).

Moving beyond this whole question about whether Carolyn described the data incorrectly, I wonder if you have any further thoughts about these empirical results and their implications. I mean that sincerely. Do people have further thoughts either regarding the statistical/empirical issues or regarding their normative significance?

the Onion Man
the Onion Man
Reply to  APDA fan
7 years ago

To be clear, I also don’t think that anyone is explicitly arguing the opposite view, mostly because it’s impossible. What I’m pointing out is rather that the commentariat here is trying desperately to avoid dealing with the superior placement numbers for women by picking a variety of nits.

Personally I think the implications are pretty clear. There is no meaningful “gender gap” in current higher education hiring. To the extent that such a “gender gap” exists, it benefits women. This fits well with sociological results robustly established elsewhere: women do better than men in high school, graduate from high school more often than men, make up a greater share of undergraduates than men, and do better in college than men. Given all of this, the results of the present study should surprise precisely no one.

Because apparently I have to spell this out, no part of the above has anything to do with historic hiring patterns. Nobody is disputing that men make up the majority of current faculty, who were hired under circumstances completely different from today’s. Nobody is claiming that those historic hiring patterns make it difficult for women in the academy. Nor does my point excuse/explain situations like the University of Miami graduating 15 men and zero women. However, it’s pretty clear that once a female student enters the Ph.D. pipeline, she is if anything structurally advantaged relative to a male student. The frantic attempts by the commantariat here to divert attention from this fact would be amusing, were it not so alarming.

postdoc
postdoc
Reply to  the Onion Man
7 years ago

I don’t understand the 54% and 49% numbers, because the total for permanent is 33% according to the table on page 16.

Anyway, that women are advantaged is consistent with what I see around here where the standards for female candidates have been dropped considerably.

the Onion Man
the Onion Man
Reply to  postdoc
7 years ago

THOUGHTCRIME

WE HAVE THOUGHTCRIME HERE

(I think the 54 and 49 are overall in the database; the 33% figure is for people who graduated between 2012 and 2015).

babygirl
babygirl
Reply to  the Onion Man
7 years ago

I think you are projecting — no one is frantically avoiding anything, they just care about this issue less than other things about the report that are interesting.

I also wouldn’t find it all that surprising if women turn out to have an advantage. But “slight advantage” does not always, or even most of the time, translate into a job. I know plenty of talented women who are struggling on the market. It’s difficult for men and women, all of whom experience frustration and disappointment, most of whom are deserving of jobs and not getting them. The terrible injustice of the job market and employment more generally in philosophy overshadow whatever slight injustice there is (if there is one, I’m not sure that there is) in some small gender advantage.

the Onion Man
the Onion Man
Reply to  babygirl
7 years ago

@babygirl

1) Perhaps “frantic” was the wrong word–perhaps not–but from where I’m sitting a lot of the comments here look like deliberate attempts to downplay or deflect attention from one of the core, highlighted, most interesting and most important results. Specifically the comments of gopher, Another gopher, and Philippe Lemoine. I appreciate your comments above engaging with them.

2) I agree with you about the relative importance of the male/female placement rates vs. the overall situation with respect to employment. Furthermore, I wouldn’t even necessarily describe the difference in placement rates as “unjust.” After all, assuming that women are doing better than men throughout every level of education for morally neutral reasons, the superior placement rate of women in higher education could be morally neutral as well: shouldn’t we expect women to emerge from the education system, on average, better qualified than men? Shouldn’t the most qualified candidate get the job? Of course, the assumptions in this line of reasoning are doing a lot of work.

Another gopher
Another gopher
Reply to  babygirl
7 years ago

@babygirl,
an 85% higher chance of receiving permanent employment, as stated in the 2015 report, is not a “slight advantage.”

Philippe Lemoine
Reply to  Another gopher
7 years ago

Actually, the previous report just gave the odds ratios obtained by the regression, it didn’t attempt to convert that into probability ratios. So 85% definitely overestimates the probability ratio for gender, which is what we are looking for. I made the same mistake at the time, because I had just skimmed through the report, instead of reading it carefully. Still, even when we transform the ratio of odds into a ratio of probabilities, the result can hardly be described as corresponding only to a “slight” advantage for women. Let me plagiarize myself by copying below something I wrote on Leiter Reports a few weeks ago on that issue:

“Based on the discussion on Daily Nous, it seems that, in the data they analyzed, being a woman increased the odds of getting a permanent position by 85%, other things being equal. However, those are *odds*, not *probabilities*. What it means in terms of probabilities will depend on the value of the other variables. But, if we take as baseline the average rate of placement in a permanent position for the whole sample, namely 53%, it corresponds to a 27.5% increase in the probability of getting a permanent position.

As I noted back then, this probably underestimates the effect of gender, since the analysis didn’t contain a variable for the number of publications and, according to previous data collected by CDJ (see this discussion: http://dailynous.com/2014/12/23/this-year-in-philosophical-intellectual-history/), men have significantly more publications on average. Thus, assuming that other things being equal having more publications increases one’s chances of getting a permanent position (which strikes me as very reasonable and, to be clear, does *not* imply that number of publications tracks merit), including a variable for number of publications in the regression model would no doubt show the effect of gender to be even stronger.”

gopher
gopher
7 years ago

Onion Man,
Nothing I said is an “attempt to downplay or deflect attention from one of the core, highlighted, most interesting and most important results.” Almost every comment I wrote *called attention* to that result.

Philippe Lemoine
Reply to  gopher
7 years ago

I was also surprised by The Onion Man’s comment, for exactly the same reason.

the Onion Man
the Onion Man
Reply to  Philippe Lemoine
7 years ago

Hi gopher and Philippe,

Apparently I at least partially misunderstood the intent of your earlier posts, for which I apologize.

anothercommenter
anothercommenter
7 years ago

It seems fairly plausible to me that the advantage for women is the result of the fact that women’s work tends to be better than men’s (particularly their written work, which is especially important on the market). And no, I don’t mean number of publications, because no search committee I’ve ever heard of makes hires by tallying numbers of pubs. They read writing samples and read them closely, as they seek quality, not quantity.

There’s a very good, cultural explanation for why women’s writing work might be deeper and more developed than men’s. Women are routinely: ignored in philosophy discussions; interrupted by men; talked over by men; patronized by men (rolling eyes, sighs, lack of proper attention to them when they’re talking); socially ostracized in grad programs b/c they’re not “one of the guys” (or b/c their attempts at friendship are met unwanted sexual advances); not invited as often to give talks and guest lectures as their male peers; when they are invited to give talks or lectures, their envious peers attribute it to affirmative action or to their attractiveness; sexually harassed; disbelieved when they report harassment (and even denounced by big names in their field); mocked for showing an interest in feminist philosophy; treated in a patronizing way by older men who regard their work as slight or probably derived from someone else; mocked for attempting to organize women-friendly or women-supportive events (despite their otherwise dismal social options); told on fora like this one that they just aren’t “good enough” to be in philosophy; and so on.

My suspicion is that all of these social barriers force women to prove themselves more. They often can’t do it in talks because they’re talked over, ignored, or are (some of them) simply anxious in these situations, having had too many bad experiences. But they can prove themselves in the slow, hard, humbling work of writing something that is actually original and not just another “Reply to M’s rejoinder to N’s objection.” My impression is that rather than do this hard work, (some) men content themselves with relatively narrow points and churn them out for publications, as though that is what good work amounts to.

This isn’t to suggest, of course, that we should not welcome a hiring advantage for women for other reasons to do with performance. I just think performance alone (again, writing quality, not quantity) might easily explain it. (To test this, we might enforce broad and strict anonymization of job dossiers and see who makes long lists. I for one am all in favor of this). Another reason we might wish to prefer women is that being a faculty member is at its core a service position, and the people served are undergrads and grad students. Many of these are women, and there are many reasons to think women students might benefit from having some decent proportion of women faculty. (Indeed, I wonder whether in the future, interpretations of Title IX might not require faculty representation of that kind). There are other reasons as well to do with diverse intellectual and social perspectives, the possibility of disrupting some of philosophy’s more anti-social (and androcentric) cultural elements, and so on. These points are, I hope, familiar.

Philippe Lemoine
Reply to  anothercommenter
7 years ago

You don’t provide any evidence for any of the claims you make about the existence of social barriers for women. I’m pretty confident that some of them are provably false. For instance, I doubt that women are less likely than men to be invited to give talks/lectures, but in any case if you think that’s true you can’t just assert it, you have to collect data and show that it’s true.

Even if you could provide data that support the claims you make about the existence of social barriers for women, you would still have to show that the existence of such barriers results in work of higher quality by women, which you also haven’t done. Moreover, given how strong the effect of gender in hiring seems to be, the effect of those social barriers would have to be even stronger, which strikes me as completely implausible.

As far as I know, the number of publications is the only independent measure of the quality of applicants we have data about and, however imperfect the data in question may be, they indicate that men are on average superior to women by that measure. To be clear, by “quality of the applicants”, I just mean how attractive they are to potential employers, regardless of whether the criteria they use to assess the merits of applications are justified.

So, even if you think that employers shouldn’t count the number of publications in favor of a candidate at all, it’s irrelevant as long as they do to some extent, which I think almost nobody would deny. Now, I understand that you deny it, but you’ll have to forgive me if I doubt that, had the situation been reversed and the data indicated that women had on average more publications than men, you would even have considered that possibility.

Thus, not only do you not offer any data to support the, to my mind, pretty wild conjecture you make, but in fact the only data we have that bear on your conjecture actually contradict it. So, if you ask me, I don’t think that you’re doing the cause that you take yourself to be defending any favor.

some person or other
some person or other
Reply to  Philippe Lemoine
7 years ago

Actually I’m pretty sure “anothercommenter” was floating a hypothesis (note the language like “It seems fairly plausible to me” and “my suspicion is that”), and suggesting a way we might empirically test that hypothesis. I’m pretty sure we’re allowed to do this in philosophy, and I’m also pretty sure that the hypothesis is not going to sound that wild to an awful lot of us.

Philippe Lemoine
Reply to  some person or other
7 years ago

Except that we’re not doing philosophy here, we’re doing social science. Or at least we’re trying, but I see that for many philosophers, it doesn’t come easy. Now, if we’re just floating hypotheses, here is another hypothesis. Suppose that all the social barriers for women that anothercommenter posits exist. It could be that, as a result of those barriers, women in graduate school get less feedback on their work, are discouraged, receive less encouragement, etc. which negatively affects the quality of their work.

In that case, given the results of the analysis performed by Carolyn and her team, the preference for women by employers would be even stronger than is suggested by the odds ratio estimated in that regression. Given the data we have, I don’t see how that hypothesis is any less plausible than anothercommenter’s if you really believe that women in philosophy face all those social barriers, but interestingly I don’t see anyone floating that particular hypothesis. A more cynical man than me might conclude that it’s because, unlike anothercommenter’s hypothesis, it doesn’t have the “right” implications.

Similarly, I very much doubt that, if the analysis had revealed a bias for men, anyone would have floated that kind of hypothesis to explain away the result, especially if the only data we had that bore on the hypothesis in question actually contradicted it. Frankly, what bothers me is not so much that women seem to be receiving preferential treatment in hiring, as much as the bad faith exhibited by some of the people who argue that they do not.

some person or other
some person or other
Reply to  Philippe Lemoine
7 years ago

actually, pretty sure now that I think about it that commenting on this blog post doesn’t constitute either an attempt to do philosophy or one to do social science, at least not necessarily so.

I’ve got nothing more to say except that:

(a) I believe that women grad students who complete their degrees are more likely to get a tt job in philosophy than male grad students who complete their degrees;
(b) we’ve got no evidence about what the explanation for this is, and your speculation (which it is) isn’t on more solid footing than the person you are taking such offense to’s;
(c) If you don’t see that women are systematically disadvantaged in professional philosophy/grad school in all sorts of other ways, then I can’t help, I’m not going to come up with some “evidence” that is going to satisfy you, and I don’t really care, because this is straightforwardly obvious to literally almost every single woman I’ve ever talked to in philosophy, successful or not, and to lots of men too;
(d) I’m super proud of the women who have achieved against these odds and I can’t see a single good reason, in the insanely bad market we are in currently, to not attempt to hire women when all else is equal—and by all accounts of people on hiring committees that I’ve heard, except for jobs with very narrow AOS’s, all else is equal, that is, there are a ton of highly qualified candidates; in fact, I think something stronger—we should aim to hire women when all else is equal;
(e) I find it just as plausible as the other commenter that women candidates are not just equally good, but better, than men, I’ve got massive amounts of systematic but, yes, anecdotal evidence that women are not taken as seriously, have to do more to prove themselves, are ignored, are talked over, are harassed, are assaulted, are marginalized, are not listened to, and that they have to compensate for this, if they are going to make it through the profession, by working extra hard;
(f) I honestly couldn’t care less whether you believe this. All I care about is what is happening in the profession: how departments are choosing to make hires; what they are valuing; how departments are choosing what students to admit; and so on. I’m grateful that there are a lot of people with power in philosophy who are choosing to try to change the face of the discipline. I haven’t seen any evidence that anyone has done anything wrong. (Indeed, I’m not even sure what it would take for a department to do something morally wrong in a hiring decision—I think we’re all generally over the idea that it’s a meritocracy, we know that people are looking for particular things and have narrow and weird sets of preferences, and yet I don’t think we have ever come to a consensus as a profession (nor, I hope, *will* we ever come to such a consensus) about how to rank and order philosophers, or candidates for a job (also, these things come apart, as has been pointed out above—there are many other things we hire for besides research and research potential).
(g) I think oftentimes the thing you describe—“women in graduate school get less feedback on their work, are discouraged, receive less encouragement, etc. which negatively affects the quality of their work”—occurs. I think women often end up leaving grad school without finishing because of the lack of support, encouragement, and feedback on their work. I think that women who make it through environments that are mostly hostile to us and then end up getting jobs in the worst job market ever should be applauded for managing to overcome all this crap. I think that people who are choosing to hire those women should be applauded. I also think that the thing that you describe starts earlier, at the undergrad level for philosophy, in many cases, and that that contributes towards women self-selecting out of philosophy and not applying to grad school.

I also think that capitalism sucks, that the job situation in philosophy and the adjunctification of universities is abysmally horrifying, and that we should be resisting *that*. I think it makes very little sense to be so actively angry/working so hard to oppose whatever (if any) preferential hiring is being done for women if you’re a man and you’re worried about getting a job, or haven’t yet gotten a job (well, it makes sense in that it is consistent with/is explained by the way that systematic oppression and sexism work that men would focus on this, but it is irrational). Women in philosophy are not the reason for this; the reason is that we are producing tons more PhDs than we have jobs for, and the reason for that is political, and much greater/has nothing to do with whether there is some affirmative action in favor of women on the job market. We all, but especially those philosophers who have secure jobs, and tenure, ought to be fighting against that. The anger at women in the profession is deeply misplaced. We’re not taking your jobs—it’s *incredibly* hard for women to get jobs too. It’s incredibly hard for anyone to get a job. Even if I have some slight advantage over you in getting a job—which is not at all clear—it’s still hard as hell for me to get a job. To be clear, I’m *not* with the feminists in philosophy who think that we should be intensely focusing, at the cost of focusing on the more important issue, on promoting women in philosophy. I think it’s great to have initiatives to do so, etc., but anyone who doesn’t think there’s a far more serious problem in the profession—the lack of jobs—is probably sitting in a serious position of privilege.

So I disagree with Onion Man here: I take the central lesson in Carolyn’s data to be about sheer numbers of/percentages of people not getting tenure track jobs. I take the central lesson to be that there are not nearly enough jobs for the I assume mostly well-qualified PhDs entering the market. That’s the issue that needs a fix. Whereas even if women *are* being given an advantage on the job market (which, like I said, I don’t actually believe), its effects on their male counterparts are miniscule compared to the effect of there just being no freaking jobs to begin with.

Philippe Lemoine
Reply to  some person or other
7 years ago

Actually, contrary to what you say, it’s clear that your explanation of the data is on more solid footing than anothercommenter’s conjecture. The data show that, even when you control for AOS and year of graduation, being a woman increases your chances of getting a permanent job significantly. Anothercommenter conjectures that it’s because women are not taken seriously and, as a result, produce superior work while they are in graduate school.

First of all, given how large the effect of gender seems to be, it would presumably have to be the case that women’s work is a *lot* better on average than men’s work. That is a very strong claim which needs to be independently supported. Now, the only data we have that independently bear on that hypothesis, namely the average number of publications for each gender, suggests that the opposite is true.

You suggest that, for anothercommenter’s hypothesis to explain the data, women’s work doesn’t have to be a lot better to men’s work, because for any particular job there are often a lot of candidates that are judged to be equally qualified. The only evidence you give for that claim is that, for any particular job, there are a lot of qualified candidates. But, from the fact that there are a lot of qualified candidates, it doesn’t follow that a lot of those are *equally* qualified.

Moreover, even putting that aside, it’s clear that anothercommenter’s hypothesis is a bad explanation of the data. As DoubleA notes below, in order to explain the data, it’s not enough that women’s work be on average better, it has to be recognized as such. But anothercommenter’s explanation of the hypothesized superiority of women’s work is that their work is generally not recognized in the profession. So it would have to be the case either that people’s bias suddenly disappears when they become part of a hiring committee, or that women’s work is so much better than men’s work that it’s enough not only to overcome that bias, but even to make them significantly more likely to get a job after the bias has been overcome.

The hypothesis that employers have a preference for women, on the other hand, doesn’t have any of those problems. In particular, it coheres with other things we know beside the hiring patterns shown in Carolyn’s data, such as the fact that a lot of people in the profession and in academia more generally are on record saying that affirmative action for women is justified.

Finally, nothing I have said implies or even suggests that I am “angry at women” and, indeed, I am not. As I already said, what really bothers me is the bad faith in this debate, which I’m sure is obvious to everyone but a handful of true believers. It’s okay if you think that women *should* get preferential treatment in hiring. I suppose that reasonable people can disagree about that. But please don’t use fallacious arguments and make unsubstantiated claims to deny that, based on the data we have, the most plausible hypothesis is that women *do* get preferential treatment in hiring, because that’s just dishonest.

A few years ago, when people were talking about the underrepresentation of women in the profession, we were told that one of the explanations was that women were less likely than men to be hired even when they were equally qualified. But now we have data that clearly show that not to be the case and, all of sudden, all sorts of ad hoc hypotheses are floated to explain the data. It’s pretty clear that, for some people, no amount of data will ever be enough to convince them of the truth of something they don’t want to accept. That’s what is annoying me.

Philippe Lemoine
Reply to  some person or other
7 years ago

“it’s clear that your explanation of the data is on more solid footing than anothercommenter’s conjecture” should of course read “it’s clear that my explanation of the data is on more solid footing than anothercommenter’s conjecture”

WP
WP
Reply to  some person or other
7 years ago

People have always been saying that women have to be better than men to get through graduate school. It may not explain the whole effect, but no, it’s definitely not ad hoc to bring it up now.

Philippe Lemoine
Reply to  some person or other
7 years ago

I’m not sure that’s true, but let’s say you’re right about that. In that case, the hypothesis may not be ad hoc, but it’s still a very poor explanation. As I already pointed out, the explanation for the alleged superiority of women in graduate school is at odds with the fact that superiority is supposed to explain, namely the fact that women are more likely to find a permanent academic position. Indeed, if the work that women produce in graduate school was not recognized, which somehow made it better, it would be unlikely to be recognized by hiring committees. It’s also the case that, so far, the only data we have that independently bear on that hypothesis, namely the data about number of publications, contradict the hypothesis that women produce better work while they are in graduate school. By the way, when those data were published, I remember that some people argued that it wasn’t surprising given that women were facing a hostile environment in graduate school. But now the hostile environment that women allegedly face is supposed to make their work better. You can’t have it both ways…

WP
WP
Reply to  some person or other
7 years ago

You’re putting a lot on anothercommenter’s particular description of the situation without considering whether there might be a better hypothesis in the same spirit, which would be normal in a less politically loaded inquiry.

I think the core of the claim is just: there are a bunch of cultural factors discouraging women from pursuing philosophy, and what makes women overcome those factors is often that they’re really good at it, enough so that women who self-select into philosophy are, on average, better than the men who do. There are also a bunch of factors that cause graduate school women to, in general, feel more of a need to prove themselves have less confidence in their work, which tends to result in stronger work.

It’s not that the value of women’s work is never recognized. It’s that women, especially in person, are less likely to be given the benefit of the doubt, less likely to be assumed to be saying something right, and so if there are ambiguities and unaddressed problems, they’re more likely to get pressed, which essentially amounts to a more rigorous graduate school experience. Because of this, women tend to spend more time per paper, and so they tend to write fewer, but better, papers. If hiring committees tend to prefer one excellent paper to two good papers (which I think is true), this would be an advantage.

Your participation here seems kind of disingenuous. “I very much doubt that, if the analysis had revealed a bias for men, anyone would have floated that kind of hypothesis to explain away the result.” Well… as you know, the plausibility of alternative explanations depends on your other evidence. Plenty of women have good reason to believe there are serious disadvantages to being a woman in philosophy.

Philippe Lemoine
Reply to  some person or other
7 years ago

First, even when you present anothercommenter’s hypothesis in that way, there is still a clear tension between the causal part of that hypothesis and the fact it’s supposed to explain. It’s just hard to see how the hypothesis could be true without it being the case that women’s applications are judged more harshly than men’s by hiring committees. But, if that is the case, it means that anothercommenter’s hypothesis is even less likely to explain the hiring data. Indeed, given how much more likely they are to find a permanent academic position, even if women’s applications were judged in exactly the same way as men’s, the quality of their work would already have to be a *lot* greater than that of men to explain the data. If on top of that women’s applications are judged more harshly, the difference between the quality of their work and that of men would have to be even greater.

But, perhaps more importantly, at the risk of repeating myself for the umpteenth time, not a shred of evidence has been given that women indeed have a harder time in graduate school or in the profession more generally. Yet, anothercommenter has made several precise claims, some of which could be tested relatively easily.

For instance, he or she claimed that women are less likely to be invited to give talks or lectures, which wouldn’t be difficult to test. I just did a little experiment to see how plausible that hypothesis looks on a relatively small sample. I looked at the people that were invited to give talks or lectures by department in Leiter’s top 20 during the academic year 2014/2015. I had to ignore almost half of them because either the data wasn’t readily available on their website or it was presented in such a way that it would have taken me forever to collect it. But I still ended up with a sample of well over 100 invited talks/lectures. In that sample, almost 39% of the people were women, even though they represent only 25% of professional philosophers or something like that.

So, based on that admittedly small sample that I collected in a totally non-rigorous way, women are actually much *more* likely than men to be invited to give talks/lectures. (You might think that it’s because women’s work is just better on average, but then your explanation is starting to look circular in a rather vicious way.) It would be easy to do that work more seriously, but I don’t see any of the people who take seriously anothercommenter’s hypothesis try, nor do I see them try to test any of the other claims he or she made. So when you tell me that *I* am the one who is being disingenuous here, you’ll have to excuse me if I don’t take you very seriously.

I agree, of course, that the plausibility of alternative explanations depends on your other evidence. My complaint is that no evidence has been given here, only completely unsubstantiated claims…

Philippe Lemoine
Reply to  some person or other
7 years ago

By the way, I have also read Quine and Duhem, so I know that if you want to save a hypothesis you can always add epicycles to do it. I don’t see why that should give us any reason to doubt the hypothesis is false unless the epicycles are independently supported by evidence.

Philippe Lemoine
Reply to  some person or other
7 years ago

Just to be clear, although I complain that people are not even trying to test the causal part of anothercommenter’s hypothesis, as I explained in response to Carolyn below, I don’t think we should focus on that. Even if that part of the hypothesis is true, it doesn’t follow that women produce on average better work and, even if it’s false, it could still be that on average their work is better. So I think we should try to test the hypothesis that women on average produce better work, without bothering to explain why that might be the case, at least until we have some evidence that suggests it *is* the case. As APDA fan says below, one relatively easy way to do that would be to look at the ratio of acceptance by gender for journals that have blind review, so in order to make that conversation more constructive I’m going to encourage people to follow APDA fan’s suggestion. To be honest, I suspect that if we do that and the results don’t confirm anothercommenter’s hypothesis, another epicycle will be added but we should do it anyway and see what happens.

Philippe Lemoine
Reply to  some person or other
7 years ago

I’m going to ask if we have the data for the Philosophical Review, which is edited at Cornell, where I’m doing my PhD. I’ll let you know what I’m told and, if the data are available, what they say.

WP
WP
Reply to  some person or other
7 years ago

I don’t know what kind of evidence you want to be given here. What people have in mind is largely anecdotal, as I think you know, and plenty of these experiences have been discussed online. You presumably don’t think very highly of the interpretations, but they’re still *some evidence.* There’s also plenty of social science research that’s been done re: how gender influences evaluations, especially women’s tendencies to underestimate their abilities. And there’s a great deal of evidence about girls tending to outperform boys in school and to spend more time on homework.

Anyway, I agree with some of your worries about anothercommenter’s proposed mechanism, but I do think it’s completely plausible that women’s work is significantly better on average, I’d guess because of differences in self selection, confidence, and time investment.

I’d be really interested to see the blind review results. anothercommenter also suggested a way we could test the claim that women’s work is, on average, better than men’s (blind review of job files), and you responded by saying “Even if you could provide data that support the claims you make about the existence of social barriers for women, you would still have to show that the existence of such barriers results in work of higher quality by women, which you also haven’t done.” This is not a normal way to engage with an alternative hypothesis!

Philippe Lemoine
Reply to  some person or other
7 years ago

Just because most of the evidence produced to argue that women face a hostile environment in philosophy is anecdotal, it doesn’t mean that we can’t test the specific claims that are made. In fact, many of the claims anothercommenter made could be tested, some of them pretty easily. I already performed a quick and dirty test of the hypothesis that women are less likely to be invited to give talks or lectures and found it to be false. It would be relatively easy to perform that test more rigorously, but again I don’t see anyone else trying.

You’re right that I don’t regard anecdotal evidence very favorably, but I don’t think that should be controversial, because the social science literature is replete with examples that show how unreliable that kind of evidence is. That’s not to say that anecdotal evidence can’t play a useful role, but I think it’s a purely heuristic role, namely the role of suggesting hypotheses for which real evidence must then be sought. But, before we test a hypothesis, we have to make sure that it’s consistent with the data we already have, which I don’t think is the case with anothercommenter’s hypothesis.

As for the suggestion that we test that hypothesis by reviewing job dossiers blindly, I think everybody knows that there is exactly zero chance this is going to happen, so making that suggestion comes pretty close to saying that we’re not going to test it. Indeed, so far, I don’t see anyone beside me doing anything to uncover evidence. I just don’t see how I’m being unfair by pointing that out, especially given the history of dishonesty in that debate. Perhaps if you were one of the only people commenting about that issue under your own name and people accused you of being angry at women for absolutely no good reason you would see things differently.

I find the kind of mechanisms you mention a lot more plausible than the ones proposed by anothercommenter. I guess I wouldn’t be entirely surprised if, because of that kind of factors, women were other things being equal more likely to produce better work. But I don’t know if other things are equal and, in any case, what I find utterly implausible is that it could explain the magnitude of the effect of gender that we observe in the data collected by Carolyn and her team.

As I already pointed out, it’s a very large effect and, moreover, we have good reasons to think it’s underestimated since the regression didn’t control for the number of publications and the data we have about that suggest men have on average significantly more publications. In light of all that, if you told me that you believe that the hypothesis that women produce on average better work than men could explain the entirety of the effect of gender in hiring or even a large part of it, I would have a hard time believing you.

But I’m willing to play ball, if we can finally put that thing to rest. We seem to agree that a good way to test that hypothesis would be to look at acceptance rates broken down by gender in journals that practice blind review. I still haven’t heard back from the editorial manager of Philosophical Review, so I don’t know if we have that information, but I will be sure to keep you posted as soon as I hear back from her. But Philosophical Review is just one journal, although a prominent one, and I don’t know if they have that information. It’s possible that most journals don’t record that information.

It would be very useful if Justin could make a post asking editors of journals who practice blind review for that information if they have it and to encourage them to collect it if they don’t. I think a lot of them read Daily Nous, so I’m sure it could be effective. Would you join me in asking Justin to write such a post? I fear that unless we actually try to uncover the evidence we need to test anothercommenter’s hypothesis we are just going to waste our time in endless disputes.

Philippe Lemoine
Reply to  some person or other
7 years ago

(Although to be fair it wasn’t anothercommenter who accused me of being angry at women.)

WP
WP
Reply to  some person or other
7 years ago

I don’t have time to fully respond right now, but I came across this and thought you’d be interested: http://dailynous.com/2014/07/08/making-philosophy-journal-statistics-publicly-available/

I appreciate you commenting under your own name, and I get that it must suck to feel like you have to do that to get your perspective out there without being dismissed as a troll. Because I think this is an important conversation, I do want to say that your first response to anothercommenter (and the same line in comments after that) reads to me like it’s in bad faith and I understand why other commenters took away that you were angry at women. The use of anecdotal evidence (etc.) here seems to me to be completely normal and appropriate, and I think some of the standards you’re applying would be really unusual on any other topic.

Didn’t post this earlier because it’s been way too long since I’ve thought about statistics and I don’t feel confident about it, but I think that, if work quality among men and women was normally distributed with the same SD, the mean work quality for women would need to be a bit less than .4 standard deviations higher than the mean for men to fully explain this effect—at .4 and with the threshold for “highly qualified” at the mean for women, 50% of women and 34% of men would be “highly qualified,” odds ratio 1.94. But… it’s been a while.

Philippe Lemoine
Reply to  some person or other
7 years ago

« I don’t have time to fully respond right now, but I came across this and thought you’d be interested: http://dailynous.com/2014/07/08/making-philosophy-journal-statistics-publicly-available/ »

Thanks. I didn’t remember that post. Assuming the acceptance rate for men and women given in that post are for the entire period covered in the data, which isn’t entirely clear but is the only way to make sense of the data, I calculated a Z-score of approximately 0.94. Even if we suppose, which I don’t think we should but whatever, that any difference in work quality between men and women could only be in favor of women, it corresponds to a p-value of approximately 0.17. So the difference in acceptance rates between men and women in the data published by Dialectica is not significant by any of the usual standards. My calculation was pretty quick and dirty, so feel free to check if you want to make sure it’s right, but the result I obtained is sufficiently unambiguous for me to be pretty confident it’s robust.

Those data certainly don’t even come close the kind of discrepancy in acceptance rates we’d expect if the mean quality of work submitted by women was 0.4 standard deviation above that of the work submitted by men. (A back-of-the-envelope calculation shows that, if we assume that only 14% of the work submitted by men is good enough for publication, that the quality of the work submitted is normally distributed for both men and women with equal variance, that in accordance with what you hypothesize in your comment women submit work whose quality is on average 0.4 standard deviation above that submitted by men and that any work above the threshold is accepted, the acceptance rate for women should be around 25%. I didn’t bother calculating a p-value because it’s clear that the observed discrepancy is highly unlikely under those assumptions. Again I may have made a mistake in my calculation so feel free to check it.)

I still haven’t heard back from the editorial manager of Philosophical Review, though I will let you know when I do. (My adviser thinks we don’t have the information, because he doesn’t think we keep track of gender/names, but we’ll see.)

« Because I think this is an important conversation, I do want to say that your first response to anothercommenter (and the same line in comments after that) reads to me like it’s in bad faith and I understand why other commenters took away that you were angry at women. The use of anecdotal evidence (etc.) here seems to me to be completely normal and appropriate, and I think some of the standards you’re applying would be really unusual on any other topic. »

I find it amazing that you would say that, because it’s pretty obvious that exactly the opposite is true. Anothercommenter proposed the hypothesis that women produce on average better work than men in order to explain the data Carolyn and her team collected. Not only did he or she didn’t give any piece of direct evidence to support that hypothesis, but the only data we have that directly bear on it, namely data about the number of publications, suggest it’s not true. In support of that hypothesis, he or she only gave a bunch of unsubstantiated claims about the mechanisms that, according to him or her, could result in the truth of that hypothesis. The only evidence he or she gave to support those claims are purely anecdotal, even though some of them at least could be tested relatively easily. For instance, I performed a quick and dirty test of the claim that women are less likely than men to be invited to give talks or lectures and found it to be false, something that could easily be done more rigorously. It’s also quite clear that it would be very easy to come up with unsubstantiated but equally plausible claims that would lead us to expect that anothercommenter’s hypothesis about the quality of work produced by women is false. Contrary to what you say, I don’t see how anyone could seriously deny that, were this debate about any other topic, the standard I’m applying would be regarded as uncontroversial. In fact, in any social science you can think of, they *are* uncontroversial.

Philippe Lemoine
Reply to  some person or other
7 years ago

« Didn’t post this earlier because it’s been way too long since I’ve thought about statistics and I don’t feel confident about it, but I think that, if work quality among men and women was normally distributed with the same SD, the mean work quality for women would need to be a bit less than .4 standard deviations higher than the mean for men to fully explain this effect—at .4 and with the threshold for “highly qualified” at the mean for women, 50% of women and 34% of men would be “highly qualified,” odds ratio 1.94. But… it’s been a while. »

I didn’t check your numbers, but off the top of my head, that sounds about right to me. However, I don’t know if we’re supposed to think that 0.4 standard deviation is a small effect, but it’s actually a pretty large one. I don’t think that you could find any study that shows a difference between men and women that even come close to such a magnitude for any cognitive ability or even for something like GPA. Moreover, in addition to the assumption that work quality is normally distributed (which is hardy obvious), your argument rest on the assumption that it has the same standard deviation among both men and women. (Normal distributions are pretty ubiquitous in nature, but that doesn’t mean that you should expect any particular distribution to be normal, unless you have good reasons to think so. Philosophy of science published a very good article by Aidan Lyon in 2013 in which he argued that the typical explanations for why distributions that are known to be normal are normal were not satisfactory.) However, if the standard deviation for men is larger than for women, even a mean difference of 0.4 standard deviation — which again is pretty large — would result in a smaller odds ratio. But there is plenty of evidence, as well as theoretical reasons to think (e. g. men only have one X chromosome), that men show more variability on cognitive abilities and a lot of other traits, so that’s hardly a trivial assumption.

Even if we accept all those assumptions, based on the only data we have so far (i. e. the ones published by Dialectica), such a difference between the mean quality of work by men and women is overwhelmingly unlikely, as I have pointed out above. The calculation I did above with the data from Dialectica using your assumptions should give you a sense of how large the kind of difference between the quality of the work of men and women would have to be in order to explain the hiring data. In fact, using the data from Dialectica makes it look a lot easier than it actually is, because over the entire period in those data the acceptance rate is unusually high for a top journal. I’m guessing that’s because in the first few years of the journal the overall acceptance rate was very high. But in the last 10 years it was about 8% and, in most top journals, it’s probably lower than 5%. Take a journal like Philosophers’ Imprint for which the acceptance rate was 4% last year according to their website. (It’s also a good choice because, since it’s an online journal with no space limitations, the way acceptance decision are made probably corresponds more closely to the simple model where anything that passes a certain threshold of quality gets published.) Let’s assume that the quality of work submitted to that journal is normally distributed with equal variance for both men and women and that only 4% of the work submitted by men passes the threshold for publication. Under those assumptions, if the quality of the work submitted by women is on average 0.4 standard deviation above that of the work submitted by men, we would expect the acceptance rate of women to be approximately 8.8%, namely more than twice the rate for men! How much would you bet that, if Philosophers’ Imprint published the acceptance rate broken down by gender, we would see anything close to that? My guess: not a lot. (But if I’m wrong please take the bet with me, I could definitely use the money.)

WP
WP
Reply to  some person or other
7 years ago

I did not take myself to be arguing anything, I just was trying to get an idea of what things would have to look like for the placement data to be explained by differences is work quality. I used a normal distribution because it seemed like the best option for a simple model, and the same SD because it was easy. I agree that .4 standard deviations is a lot.

And yes, that was my interpretation as well re: the journal data. I guess given the gender differences in AOS we should be asking about how journal AOS preferences compare to job AOS preferences.

Anyway, I’m most familiar with psychology, but something you see pretty often in psychology journals:
paper: I got results. Here’s an interpretation.
comment on paper: You haven’t ruled out this alternative interpretation, which is more consistent with what a lot of people believe. Evidence X may seem to suggest against this alternative interpretation, but there are reasons to doubt it.
reply to comment: o. Here’s some additional evidence.

Something you don’t see often:
paper: I got results. Here’s an interpretation.
comment on paper: You haven’t ruled out this alternative interpretation, which is more consistent with what a lot of people believe. Evidence X may seem to suggest against this alternative interpretation, but there are reasons to doubt it.
reply to comment: You haven’t provided any evidence to show those common beliefs are true. You haven’t proved your alternative interpretation. The only piece of evidence we have, Evidence X, suggests against it.

???

Philippe Lemoine
Reply to  some person or other
7 years ago

« Anyway, I’m most familiar with psychology, but something you see pretty often in psychology journals:
paper: I got results. Here’s an interpretation.
comment on paper: You haven’t ruled out this alternative interpretation, which is more consistent with what a lot of people believe. Evidence X may seem to suggest against this alternative interpretation, but there are reasons to doubt it.
reply to comment: o. Here’s some additional evidence. »

I agree that you see that often, but it also has little to do with what’s going on here. When in a scientific journal someone points out that an alternative explanation H that is more consistent with X, where X is something a lot of people in the field believe, has not been ruled out, X is not just anything that people happen to believe. Rather, X is something that is taken to be well supported by evidence, not anecdotal evidence but serious evidence. For instance, X is a popular theory, which has been successfully tested in a variety of ways. It doesn’t matter if a lot of people believe something unless they have good reasons to do so. There are plenty of examples of things which a lot of people, including a lot of philosophers, believe even though they are false. So perhaps a lot of people believe that women face a hostile environment in philosophy, but I can’t say that I’ve seen a lot of non-anecdotal evidence to that effect. (Warning: this was an understatement.) When I performed a quick and dirty test of one precise claim made by anothercommenter, namely the claim that women are less likely to be invited to give talks or lectures, I found it to be false. If people want to test that hypothesis more rigorously, they are welcome to do so and share the results with us.

Moreover, it also has to be the case that we have good reasons to think that H really is more consistent with X, which again requires more than mere speculation. One can speculate that, if women face a hostile environment in philosophy, it makes their work better. One can probably even find a few studies that lend some very indirect support to that claim. But then again one can also speculate that, if women face a hostile environment in philosophy, it negatively impacts the quality of their work. And one can surely find studies that lend some very indirect support to that claim. Scientists usually require that the connection between X and H be less ambiguous in order to consider it a serious problem that H was not ruled out. It’s not enough that you be able to cook up a somewhat plausible story to the effect that X makes H plausible, especially when you can just as easily cook up an equally plausible story to the effect that X makes H implausible. That’s even more true when H, as the hypothesis that women on average produce better work, is actually at odds with the only data we have that directly bear on it.

Finally, I don’t think a lot of scientists would take seriously the complaint that H has not been ruled out, unless we have good reasons that H could explain the data. But, as the back-of-the-envelope calculations I did above suggest, given the magnitude of the effect of gender in Carolyn’s data, it’s rather unlikely that the hypothesis that women produce on average better work could explain it. Again, if one really wants to deny that something is true, there is always a way to do so.

Anyway, I feel that we’re past the point where this discussion is likely to remain productive, so I’ll just let you have the last word if you want it. I’ll just summarize once more what I take to be the situation. Anothercommenter suggested that the hypothesis that women produce on average better work than men could explain the fact that women are more likely to find a permanent academic position even if employers didn’t have any preference for women. I think that hypothesis is no more plausible than the contrary hypothesis that men produce on average better work than women and that neither of them is particularly likely. The only evidence we have for the moment that bear directly to anothercommenter’s hypothesis, namely Carolyn’s data about number of publications and Dialectica’s data about acceptance rates broken down by gender, clearly speaks against it. Moreover, a few back-of-the-envelope calculations suggest that even if that hypothesis is true, it’s very unlikely to explain the magnitude of the effect of gender in the data or even to come close to that. If people still think that’s a promising avenue to refute the hypothesis that employers have a preference for women, I’m really not sure what else I could tell them that would change their mind.

As for the question of who is arguing in good faith in this debate and who isn’t, I will let the people who read this make up their own mind. If they care to do so, I invite them to consider whether I have ever changed my standards since I first talked about that issue back in 2014, how often I had to change my hypothesis and add epicycles to it in order to accommodate inconvenient evidence, whether I’m commenting under my own name and therefore taking responsibility for everything I say, how many times I took the time to seriously engage with the data and perform statistical analyses on it, etc. Then, if they want, they can ask themselves the same questions about the people I have been up against. I can’t say that I’m overly worried about what lesson most people will draw from that comparison.

Philippe Lemoine
Reply to  some person or other
7 years ago

Just one last note to let you know that I just heard back from the editorial manager of Philosophical Review and unfortunately we don’t have the information about the acceptance rate broken down by gender.

GhostWriteDaQuip
GhostWriteDaQuip
Reply to  Philippe Lemoine
7 years ago

I think Anothercommenter was describing a subset of reactions that people (in this case, women) might have to the situations described. You’re describing another subset of reactions to those situations. I think it’s uncontroversial to say that sometimes people react the way Anothercommenter describes. other times, the way you describe. The same person might even react one way at one point, and differently later on.

I think the point was that sometimes women have to be really tough and do really good work to make it through the gauntlet of being a woman in philosophy. Those women produce really good work.

Like T.I.P. says, what don’t kill you…

But anyway, I think you’re right that there would have been a different explanation if the bias went the other way. The data needs to be squared with the claim that sexism is a real thing. That’s a claim supported by a lot of other evidence- like that which Anothercommenter describes.

I think that’s what happened anyway.

Philippe Lemoine
Reply to  GhostWriteDaQuip
7 years ago

“I think Anothercommenter was describing a subset of reactions that people (in this case, women) might have to the situations described. You’re describing another subset of reactions to those situations. I think it’s uncontroversial to say that sometimes people react the way Anothercommenter describes. other times, the way you describe. The same person might even react one way at one point, and differently later on.”

No. Anothercommenter’s was proposing an alternative explanation of the data. For his/her hypothesis to explain the data, it’s not enough that *some* women react to the social barriers anothercommenter’s posits in that way, while others react in the other way I proposed. It has to be the case that the kind of reaction anothercommenter’s posits be the overwhelmingly dominant reaction and, even if those social barriers exist (no evidence was given that they do), we have been given no reason to believe that it would be the case.

“But anyway, I think you’re right that there would have been a different explanation if the bias went the other way. The data needs to be squared with the claim that sexism is a real thing. That’s a claim supported by a lot of other evidence- like that which Anothercommenter describes.”

What evidence? As far as I can tell, anothercommenter didn’t give a shred of evidence for any of the claims he or she made. Yet some of those claims would be relatively easy to test. (Others, not so much. Others are in fact contradicted by the data we have.) For instance, take the claim that women are less likely than men to be invited to give talks/lectures. It would not be very difficult to collect data to test that hypothesis. If people really believe that it’s true, why don’t they do so?

Carolyn Dicey Jennings
Carolyn Dicey Jennings
Reply to  GhostWriteDaQuip
7 years ago

“For his/her hypothesis to explain the data, it’s not enough that *some* women react to the social barriers anothercommenter’s posits in that way, while others react in the other way I proposed. It has to be the case that the kind of reaction anothercommenter’s posits be the overwhelmingly dominant reaction and, even if those social barriers exist (no evidence was given that they do), we have been given no reason to believe that it would be the case.”

One possibility, mentioned above, is that the described reaction is not the dominant one per se but the dominant one among the “survivors”, i.e. those who graduate despite the harsher conditions. Here is one study I know of that looked at the different characteristics of women in these environments (in athletics), but I suspect there are more: http://www2.ne.su.se/paper/wp07_03.pdf Another anecdote: I played water polo in college (at the University of St Andrews) and the women’s team practiced with the men’s team. Whereas the men’s team was pretty good, the women’s team was great–we won the Scottish Cup in 2003. (To be fair, I was perhaps the worst player on that team and I think the coach put me in the water for our winning game out of sympathy so that I could wear a medal.) I think this was in part because of the more intense environment of practicing with people who dramatically outweighed us. In any case, I think anonthercommenter’s hypothesis is interesting and worth considering. (And testing!)

Philippe Lemoine
Reply to  GhostWriteDaQuip
7 years ago

Anothercommenter’s hypothesis is that women in philosophy face various social barriers that men don’t and, as a result, they produce work that is on average superior to that of men, enough to explain the data you have collected. Note first that, even if women really face such barriers and, because of them, they produce work that is on average superior to that of men, it doesn’t follow that it’s enough to explain the data. Indeed, according to the data, women are much more likely than men to find a permanent academic position even after controlling for AOS and the year of graduation, so presumably the work of women would have to be a *lot* better on average than that of men.

In fact, even if women face social barriers that make them produce better work, it’s entirely possible that the average quality of their work is nevertheless *inferior* to that of men because of other factors. So I don’t think we should try to test anothercommenter’s hypothesis, because even if the causal part of that hypothesis was true, it wouldn’t by itself tell us anything about what we’re trying to know. Moreover, that kind of causal hypothesis would be extremely difficult to test, if not impossible. Finally, as I noted above, the causal part of the hypothesis can already seen to be very implausible. Indeed, it’s not enough that women produce work that is on average superior in order to explain why they are more likely to find a permanent academic position, it has to be recognized as such. But, according to the first part of anothercommenter’s hypothesis, the quality of the work produced by women is precisely supposed *not* to be recognized.

So, for the moment, we should just focus on the part of anothercommenter’s hypothesis that is supposed to do the explanatory work, namely the claim that women on average produce better work than men. I’m sure I could also find studies which suggest that women perform less well when they are placed in a hostile environment, but I don’t really see the point in engaging in that kind of speculation until evidence that women on average produce better work than men has been given. And, so far, the only data we have that independently bear on that hypothesis, namely the data you collected a few years ago about the number of publications, suggest that exactly the opposite is true.

If anothercommenter or anybody else nevertheless thinks that women on average produce better work than men, they collect data and show that it’s indeed the case. If they can show that, they should argue that the difference is sufficient to explain the data about hiring, which as I already noted is another kettle of fish. As APDA fan pointed out below, it’s something that could totally be done, if people really want to find out the truth. Perhaps they will even be able to confirm that hypothesis, although I highly doubt that, even if they can show that women on average produce better work, it will be enough to explain the data about hiring, which show that women have a very substantial advantage. In any case, until someone has done that, it will remain the case that, as far as we can tell, the best explanation for the data about hiring is that employers have a preference for women. I’m sorry but I just don’t see how anyone could deny that in good faith.

APDA fan
APDA fan
Reply to  anothercommenter
7 years ago

anothercommenter,

I would be interested in discussing more concrete ways of testing your hypothesis that “women’s work tends to be better than men’s (particularly their written work, which is especially important on the market).” Suppose we look at submissions to blind review journals, where the reviewers typically do not know the gender of the author. Would it be possible to test this hypothesis by checking to see whether there was a tendency whereby submissions by women were accepted more frequently than submissions by men at journals of this type?

Anothersomeone
Anothersomeone
Reply to  APDA fan
7 years ago

Yes that sounds like a good test, with the important caveat that currently, much reviewing is unfortunately not anonymous bc of unscrupulous referee googling. How common this is, I have no idea, but I’ve had at least one person who should dfn know better tell me quite shamelessly that they do it when they referee.

Anothersomeone
Anothersomeone
Reply to  APDA fan
7 years ago

I should add that the best test of whether women’s best work fares better than men’s best work could also be tested, as I mentioned, by looking at who makes job long lists in a world where all job dossiers are wholly anonymous (and where letters aren’t used, at least at that stage). I’m assuming writing samples generally represent one’s best work.

Anothersomeone
Anothersomeone
Reply to  Anothersomeone
7 years ago

*a test, not “the best test”

the Onion Man
the Onion Man
7 years ago

@some person or other

1) “if women *are* being given an advantage on the job market (which, like I said, I don’t actually believe)”

This is exactly the kind of double standard on the Left that is so utterly infuriating I feel I have no choice but to descend into hostile environments like this blog and at least attempt to set the record straight. Here you are presented with a study that is practically slapping you in the face with concrete data to the effect that there is no “gender gap” in TT hiring, and to the extent there is one it benefits women. This is not some neckbeard MRA complaining in 4chan, this is new data in the first quantitative study of its kind, organized by a female professor. And yet you still insist that it can’t possibly be accurate, because it contravenes what you just know in your heart of hearts to be true and your aggrieved sense of “social justice.” I’d like you to pause for a moment and consider what might happen if the situation were reversed, particularly in terms of the prestige-granting effects of “science.”

2) “I also think that capitalism sucks”

“we are producing tons more PhDs than we have jobs for”

So let me get this straight. Supply far outstrips demand, yet the problem is with capitalism? If you recognize that a situation where you have far more supply than demand for a particular kind of labor (say academic labor) will both lower the price of that labor and make it more difficult to find for those inclined to perform it, you have conceded that capitalism is working. If your proposed solution to this problem is to prop up the academic labor market with taxpayer money, you are delusional.

As noted above I largely agree that the overall job market situation is more of an issue than the (at present) slight advantage possessed by women in hiring decisions. But your resolute insistence on avoiding the plain facts of the matter does absolutely no one any favors.

DoubleA
DoubleA
7 years ago

I’m not sure how to square the hypothesis that a bias toward women in tt hiring (if there is any) is the result of their work being on average better than the male competition, with the evidenc suggesting that mens work is typically evaluated more favorably than women’s. To explain the hiring pattern, it’s not enough that the work of women is better. The work as must be recognized as better by the people doing the hiring, but the oft cited data on implicit bias suggests this is unlikely. I suppose it’s possible that the work of women is so much better that it overcomes the implicit bias, but this seems like a very strong empirical claim– one inconsistent with the claim that often things are equal between job candidates.

DoubleA
DoubleA
Reply to  DoubleA
7 years ago

I should add that although googling work that one is reviewing is all too common, the odds of genuinely blind peer review are still much higher than the odds of a “blind search,” especially at the point in the search where writing samples are read. You would then expect a greater anti-woman bias in hiring than what you see in peer review. None of this tells you if a pro-woman bias in hiring is justified, nor does it conclusively show that there is such a bias. But I don’t see how the quality of work explanation can be maintained.

APDA fan
APDA fan
7 years ago

It is sad that our discussion of this gender result has become so antagonistic. Let’s go back to thinking about how to get to the bottom of the relevant empirical issues.

The commenters above suggest two different ways of testing the hypothesis that women’s written work is simply of higher quality.:

One is to have people review job applications while remaining blind to the gender of the applicant. This is an elegant research design, but of course, it would require a great deal of further work to actually implement it.

The other is to look at the frequency of acceptance at blind review journals for submissions from philosophers of either gender. Unlike the first suggestion, this would require very little further work. Editors of such journals already have the data we need, and all that is required is for them to reveal these frequencies.

Do any readers have access to this information? Or does anyone have any ideas about how to obtain it?

W Hughes
W Hughes
7 years ago

I’m a scientist, not a philosopher, but I read your paper and would like to congratulate you on gathering a very comprehensive dataset. The perennial underrepresentation of women in philosophy certainly needs a quantitative exploration of the underlying structural factors.

The data you present is fairly convincing as is, but I was wondering if you had thought about putting together a more comprehensive statistical model to explore the data in greater depth. For instance, rather than running multiple simple tests (e.g. chi-square tests), it might make more sense to use a multi-factor model with interaction terms; that way you control for the variance in the response explained by factor 1 given the state of factor 2 and vice versa. Practically, this might help answer some of your questions, e.g. “does area of specialization matter equally for male and female philosophers? Furthermore, it might be worth parsing out some of the residual variance that could be accounted for by the blocking of your data into classes that have little or nothing to do with the response (e.g. years); by including these “nuisance factors” into a fuller model, you get a more powerful test of your hypothesis.

I’m not trying to criticize–I really liked the paper. I just wanted to suggest a few minor changes to the statistical analyses that might allow you to treat your hypotheses more directly (rather than using simple tests, which are only valid if a number of assumptions are met).

Anyway, thanks again for the read.