Social Psych in the Philosophy Classroom


Normally, self-affirmation is reserved for instances in which identity is threatened in direct ways: race, gender, age, weight, and the like. Here, Nyhan decided to apply it in an unrelated context: Could recalling a time when you felt good about yourself make you more broad-minded about highly politicized issues, like the Iraq surge or global warming? As it turns out, it would. On all issues, attitudes became more accurate with self-affirmation, and remained just as inaccurate without. That effect held even when no additional information was presented—that is, when people were simply asked the same questions twice, before and after the self-affirmation.

The question of why people persistently hold onto false beliefs has long been of interest to philosophers, and has received increased attention in the social sciences. An article in the New Yorker summarizes some recent experimental work on correcting false beliefs. As it turns out, providing correct information to the false believers doesn’t work all that well. What does? According to the work of political scientist Brendan Nyhan (Dartmouth), which is summarized in the article, this: getting people to feel good about themselves.

The philosophy classroom is a challenging place. The methods are unfamiliar, the readings difficult, and, most directly, it is often the goal of philosophy professors to problematize the ordinary, that is, to get people to see that the beliefs they take for granted may be unjustified or rooted in mistakes or in need of clarification and defense. Many students are resistant to this and are turned off by philosophy.

Does Nyhan’s work provide a lesson for philosophy professors? Will our students be more receptive to criticism and correction, more “broad-minded”, if we first make sure they feel good about themselves? How could we do that?

And what other findings should philosophy professors tailor their teaching techniques in light of? If students are more responsive if they are fonder of their professors, perhaps we should offer them warm drinks. Or maybe we should adjust the colors of our handouts, or presentation slides, or classroom walls, depending on the kind of assignments we are asking the students to do. Or perhaps to come off as more authoritative and competent, we should stand up straight, lecture on a raised platform, and do other things to make ourselves seem taller.

It would be interesting to learn if philosophy teachers have had pedagogical success by paying attention to their students’ cognitive biases and heuristics, taking advantage of their automatic responses to certain stimuli, or otherwise nudging them.

(Note: slow comment approval warning still in effect.)

There are 2 comments

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

  
Please enter an e-mail address